Why We Read Fiction : Theory of Mind and the Novel /
Why We Read Fiction offers a lucid overview of the most exciting area of research in contemporary cognitive psychology known as "Theory of Mind" and discusses its implications for literary studies. It covers a broad range of fictional narratives, from Richardson s Clarissa, Dostoyevski...
Saved in:
Main Author: | |
---|---|
Format: | Electronic eBook |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Columbus :
The Ohio State University Press,
[2006]
|
Series: | Book collections on Project MUSE.
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | Full text available: |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
MARC
LEADER | 00000cam a22000004a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
001 | musev2_28189 | ||
003 | MdBmJHUP | ||
005 | 20240815120737.0 | ||
006 | m o d | ||
007 | cr||||||||nn|n | ||
008 | 170203t20062006ohu o 00 0 eng d | ||
010 | |z 2005028358 | ||
020 | |a 9780814272633 | ||
020 | |z 0814272630 | ||
020 | |z 081425151X | ||
020 | |z 9780814251515 | ||
035 | |a (OCoLC)971252586 | ||
040 | |a MdBmJHUP |c MdBmJHUP | ||
100 | 1 | |a Zunshine, Lisa, |e author. | |
245 | 1 | 0 | |a Why We Read Fiction : |b Theory of Mind and the Novel / |c Lisa Zunshine. |
264 | 1 | |a Columbus : |b The Ohio State University Press, |c [2006] | |
264 | 3 | |a Baltimore, Md. : |b Project MUSE, |c 2021 | |
264 | 4 | |c ©[2006] | |
300 | |a 1 online resource (200 pages): |b illustrations. | ||
336 | |a text |b txt |2 rdacontent | ||
337 | |a computer |b c |2 rdamedia | ||
338 | |a online resource |b cr |2 rdacarrier | ||
490 | 0 | |a Theory and interpretation of narrative | |
505 | 0 | 0 | |g pt. 1. |t Attributing minds. Why did Peter Walsh tremble? -- |t What is mind-reading (also known as theory of mind)? -- |t Theory of mind, autism, and fiction : four caveats -- |t "Effortless" mind-reading -- |t Why do we read fiction? -- |t The novel as a cognitive experiment -- |t Can cognitive science tell us why we are afraid of Mrs. Dalloway? -- |t The relationship between a "cognitive" analysis of Mrs. Dalloway and the larger field of literary studies -- |t Woolf, Pinker, and the project of interdisciplinarity -- |g pt. 2. |t Tracking minds. Whose thought is it, anyway? -- |t Metarepresentational ability and schizophrenia -- |t Everyday failures of source-monitoring -- |t Monitoring fictional states of mind -- |t "Fictional" and "history" -- |t Tracking minds in Beowulf -- |t Don Quixote and his progeny -- |t Source-monitoring, ToM, and the figure of the unreliable narrator -- |t Source-monitoring and the implied author -- |t Richardson's Clarissa : the progress of the elated bridegroom -- |t Nabokov's Lolita : the deadly demon meets and destroys the tenderhearted boy -- |g pt. 3. |t Concealing minds. ToM and the detective novel : what does it take to suspect everybody? -- |t Why is reading a detective story a lot like lifting weights at the gym? -- |t Metarepresentationality and some recurrent patterns of the detective story -- |t A cognitive evolutionary perspective : always historicize! -- |g Conclusion : |t why do we read (and write) fiction? Authors meet their readers -- |t Is this why we read fiction? surely, there is more to it! |
506 | 0 | |a Open Access |f Unrestricted online access |2 star | |
520 | |a Why We Read Fiction offers a lucid overview of the most exciting area of research in contemporary cognitive psychology known as "Theory of Mind" and discusses its implications for literary studies. It covers a broad range of fictional narratives, from Richardson s Clarissa, Dostoyevski's Crime and Punishment, and Austen s Pride and Prejudice to Woolf's Mrs. Dalloway, Nabokov's Lolita, and Hammett s The Maltese Falcon. Zunshine's surprising new interpretations of well-known literary texts and popular cultural representations constantly prod her readers to rethink their own interest in fictional narrative. Written for a general audience, this study provides a jargon-free introduction to the rapidly growing interdisciplinary field known as cognitive approaches to literature and culture. | ||
588 | |a Description based on print version record. | ||
650 | 7 | |a Ciência cognitiva. |2 larpcal | |
650 | 7 | |a Ficção (gênero) |2 larpcal | |
650 | 7 | |a Literatura. |2 larpcal | |
650 | 1 | 7 | |a Cognitieve processen. |2 gtt |
650 | 1 | 7 | |a Psychologische aspecten. |2 gtt |
650 | 1 | 7 | |a Lezen. |2 gtt |
650 | 1 | 7 | |a Fictie. |2 gtt |
650 | 7 | |a Literaturpsychologie |2 gnd | |
650 | 7 | |a Fiction |x Psychological aspects. |2 fast |0 (OCoLC)fst00923742 | |
650 | 7 | |a Fiction. |2 fast |0 (OCoLC)fst00923709 | |
650 | 7 | |a Cognitive science. |2 fast |0 (OCoLC)fst00866547 | |
650 | 7 | |a Books and reading. |2 fast |0 (OCoLC)fst00836454 | |
650 | 7 | |a LITERARY CRITICISM / General |2 bisacsh | |
650 | 6 | |a Sciences cognitives. | |
650 | 6 | |a Livres et lecture. | |
650 | 6 | |a Roman |x Aspect psychologique. | |
650 | 6 | |a Roman. | |
650 | 0 | |a Cognitive science. | |
650 | 0 | |a Books and reading. | |
650 | 0 | |a Fiction |x Psychological aspects. | |
650 | 0 | |a Fiction. | |
655 | 7 | |a Romans. |2 rvmgf | |
655 | 7 | |a Fiction. |2 lcgft | |
655 | 7 | |a Fiction. |2 fast |0 (OCoLC)fst01423787 | |
655 | 2 | |a Fictional Work | |
655 | 7 | |a Electronic books. |2 local | |
710 | 2 | |a Project Muse. |e distributor | |
830 | 0 | |a Book collections on Project MUSE. | |
856 | 4 | 0 | |z Full text available: |u https://muse.jhu.edu/book/28189/ |
999 | |c 231500 |d 231499 |